Abstract mindset favors well-being and reduces risk behaviors for adolescents in relative scarcity

Main Article Content

Amparo Caballero
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5809-0940
Bronwyn Laforêt
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7163-6458
Pilar Carrera
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3940-9028

Abstract

Previous research has shown that when people perceive themselves to be experiencing financial scarcity, their thinking style becomes more concrete. Construal level theory points out that thinking in a more concrete way implies focusing more on the short-term consequences of behaviors and is associated with contextual difficulties, less self-control, and increased engagement in risk behaviors. The aim of this study was to understand the factors that affect subjective well-being and engagement in risk behaviors in adolescents with high perceived relative economic scarcity. A total of 463 students (aged 12 to 18), 264 females, from public schools in Madrid (Spain) responded to the study questionnaire. Adolescents with higher perceived relative economic scarcity presented a more concrete thinking style and lower subjective well-being and reported more past risk behaviors and greater intentions to engage in risk behaviors in the future. Participants with a more concrete thinking style and high perceived relative economic scarcity presented the lowest subjective well-being and most risk behaviors, compared to those with a more abstract thinking style and high perceived relative economic scarcity, and either thinking style and low perceived relative economic scarcity. School-based interventions should be developed for vulnerable adolescents, especially those with high perceived relative economic scarcity, focusing on promoting a more abstract thinking style, to reduce engagement in risk behaviors and increase subjective well-being. Results highlight the vulnerability (lower subjective well-being and higher risk behaviors) for adolescents who feel in a situation of high perceived relative economic scarcity compared to peers. However, results also indicate that an abstract thinking style can serve as a protective factor, suggesting a new intervention approach in adolescent health.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Article Details

How to Cite
Caballero, A., Laforêt, B., & Carrera, P. (2024). Abstract mindset favors well-being and reduces risk behaviors for adolescents in relative scarcity. Psychology, Society & Education, 16(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.21071/pse.v16i1.16182
Section
Artículos
Author Biographies

Amparo Caballero, Autónoma University of Madrid, Madrid, (Spain)

PhD in Psychology. University Professor. Department of Social Psychology and Methodology. Faculty of Psychology. Autónoma University of Madrid.

Bronwyn Laforêt, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid (Spain)

PhD Student in Psychology. Autónoma University of Madrid

Pilar Carrera, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid (Spain)

PhD in Psychology. University Professor. Department of Social Psychology and Methodology. Faculty of Psychology. Autónoma University of Madrid.

References

Aguilar, P., Caballero, A., Sevillano, V., Fernández, I., Muñoz, D., & Carrera, P. (2020). The relationships between economic scarcity, concrete mindset and risk behavior: A study of Nicaraguan adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(11), Article 3845. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17113845 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17113845

Atienza, F. L., Pons, D., Balaguer, I., & García-Merita, M. (2000). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Satisfacción con la Vida en adolescentes. Psicothema, 12(2), 314-319.

Caballero, A., Fernández, I., Aguilar, P., & Carrera, P. (2022). The links among relative financial scarcity, thinking style, fatalism, and well-being. PsyCh Journal, 11(6), 885-894. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.566 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.566

Caballero, A., Fernández, I., Aguilar, P., Muñoz, D., & Carrera, P. (2023). Does poverty promote a different and harmful way of thinking? The links between economic scarcity, concrete construal level and risk behaviors. Current Psychology, 42(10), 8402-8413. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02382-3 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02382-3

Carrera, P., Aguilar, P., Fernández, I., & Caballero, A. (2022). Health or wealth? The influence of perceived health and wealth threats and style of thinking on protective behaviours and well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic in Spain. International Journal of Social Psychology, 38(1), 66-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2022.2132744 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2022.2132744

Carrera, P., Fernández, I., Muñoz, D., & Caballero, A. (2020). Using abstractness to confront challenges: How the abstract construal level increases people’s willingness to perform desirable but demanding actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(2), 339-349. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000244 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000244

Carrera, P., Laforêt, B., & Caballero, A. (2023, October 4-6). Influence of the abstract thinking style on subjective well-being [Roundtable presentation]. World Congress of Violence in School, Seville, Spain.

Carrera, P., Muñoz, D., Fernández, I., & Caballero, A. (2018). Abstractness and messages describing consequences promote healthier behavioral intentions. The Journal of Psychology, 152(7), 515-527. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2018.1486801 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2018.1486801

Carrera, P., Sheppard, V. B., Caballero, A., Gómez-Trillos, S., & Hurtado-de-Mendoza, A. (2022). A culturally targeted video to promote genetic counseling in a community sample of at-risk US Latina women: The role of the concrete mindset. Journal of Community Psychology, 50(3), 1331-1342. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.22718 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/jcop.22718

Cheung, F., & Lucas, R. E. (2016). Income inequality is associated with stronger social comparison effects: The effect of relative income on life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(2), Article 332. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000059 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000059

Chiou, W. B., Wu, W. H., & Chang, M. H. (2013). Think abstractly, smoke less: A brief construal-level intervention can promote self-control, leading to reduced cigarette consumption among current smokers. Addiction, 108(5), 985-992. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12100 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12100

De Botton, A. (2004). Status Anxiety. Penguin Publishers.

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The Satisfaction with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71-75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), Article 15. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115

Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2005). Income and well-being: An empirical analysis of the comparison income effect. Journal of Public Economics, 89(5-6), 997-1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2004.06.003 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2004.06.003

Fujita, K., & Han, H. A. (2009). Moving beyond deliberative control of impulses: The effect of construal levels on evaluative associations in self-control conflicts. Psychological Science, 20(7), 799-804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x

Fujita, K., Orvell, A., & Kross, E. (2020). Smarter, not harder: A toolbox approach to enhancing self-control. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(2), 149-156. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732220941242 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732220941242

Gilbert, K. M., Weissman, D., Gordils, J., Elliot, A., & Sommet, N. (2023). Feeling poor or feeling poorer: Comparing subjective measures of income in predicting health and well-being. International Journal of Social Psychology, 38(3), 541-569. https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2023.2236468 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2023.2236468

Iacobucci, D., Posavac, S. S., Kardes, F. R., Schneider, M. J., & Popovich, D. L. (2015). Toward a more nuanced understanding of the statistical properties of a median split. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(4), 652-665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2014.12.002 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2014.12.002

MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 19-40. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.19 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.19

Melita, D., Willis, G. B., & Rodríguez-Bailón, R. (2021). Economic inequality increases status anxiety through perceived contextual competitiveness. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 637365. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637365 DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.637365

Moya, M., & Alcañiz-Colomer, J. (2023) The different effects of income and educational level on prosocial behaviour and related ideological variables. International Journal of Social Psychology, 38(3), 508-540. https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2023.2229171 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2023.2229171

Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2014). Scarcity: The new science of having less and how it defines our lives. Macmillan Publishers.

Nishi, A., Shirado, H., Rand, D. G., & Christakis, N. A. (2015). Inequality and visibility of wealth in experimental social networks. Nature, 526(7573), 426-429. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15392 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15392

Payne, B. K., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Hannay, J. W. (2017). Economic inequality increases risk taking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(18), 4643-4648. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1616453114 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1616453114

Richter, M. (2016). Residual tests in the analysis of planned contrasts: Problems and solutions. Psychological Methods, 21, 112-120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000044 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000044

Rosnow, R. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1989). Definition and interpretation of interaction effects. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 143-146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.1.143 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.105.1.143

Sánchez-Rodríguez, Á., Jetten, J., Willis, G., & Rodríguez-Bailón, R, et al. (2019). High economic inequality makes us feel less wealthy. International Review of Social Psychology, 32(1), Article 17, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.333 DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.333

Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2012). Some consequences of having too little. Science, 338(6107), 682-685. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1222426 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1222426

Sommet, N., Morselli, D., & Spini, D. (2018). Income inequality affects the psychological health of only the people facing scarcity. Psychological Science, 29(12), 1911-1921. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797618798620 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618798620

Sweeney, A. M., & Freitas, A. L. (2014). Relating action to abstract goals increases physical activity reported a week later. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15(4), 364-373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.03.009 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.03.009

Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403-421. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403

Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963

United Nations Children’s Fund. (2018). Programme guidance for early life prevention of non-communicable diseases. https://www.unicef.org/health/adolescent-health-and-well-being

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660-671. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.57.4.660

Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (2012). Action identification theory. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology (pp. 327-348). Sage Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n17 DOI: https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446249215.n17

Villar S., Carrera, P., & Oceja, L. (2022). From aesthetics to ethics: Testing the link between an emotional experience of awe and the motive of quixoteism on (un)ethical behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 46(4), 508-520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-022-09935-4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-022-09935-4

White, K., MacDonnell, R., & Dahl, D. W. (2011). It’s the mind-set that matters: The role of construal level and message framing in influencing consumer efficacy and conservation behaviors. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3), 472-485. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.3.472 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.3.472

World Health Organization. (2014). Health for the world’s adolescents: A second chance in the second decade. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/112750